ABOUT THE SPEAKER
Alan Kay - Educator and computing pioneer
One of the true luminaries of personal computing, Alan Kay conceived of laptops and graphical interfaces years before they were realized. At XeroxPARC, Apple, HP and Disney, he has developed tools for improving the mind.

Why you should listen

"The best way to predict the future is to invent it." Alan Kay not only coined this favorite tech-world adage, but has proven its truth several times. A true polymath, as well as inventor, he has combined engineering brilliance with knowledge of child development, epistemology, molecular biology and more.

In the 1960s, Kay joined the computer team at XeroxPARC, where he worked on world-changing inventions like the graphical interface, object-oriented programming, and the personal computer itself. Later, at Apple, Atari, HP, Disney, and now at his own nonprofits, he has helped refine the tools he anticipated long before they were realized.

As the industry has blossomed, however, Kay continues to grapple with the deeper purpose of computing, struggling to create the machine that won't only recapitulate patterns in the world as we know it but will teach both children and adults to think, to see what otherwise is beyond them.

More profile about the speaker
Alan Kay | Speaker | TED.com
TED2007

Alan Kay: A powerful idea about ideas

Filmed:
851,991 views

With all the intensity and brilliance for which he is known, Alan Kay envisions better techniques for teaching kids by using computers to illustrate experience in ways -– mathematically and scientifically -- that only computers can.
- Educator and computing pioneer
One of the true luminaries of personal computing, Alan Kay conceived of laptops and graphical interfaces years before they were realized. At XeroxPARC, Apple, HP and Disney, he has developed tools for improving the mind. Full bio

Double-click the English transcript below to play the video.

00:18
A great way to start, I think, with my view of simplicity
0
0
4000
00:22
is to take a look at TED. Here you are, understanding why we're here,
1
4000
7000
00:29
what's going on with no difficulty at all.
2
11000
5000
00:34
The best A.I. in the planet would find it complex and confusing,
3
16000
4000
00:38
and my little dog Watson would find it simple and understandable
4
20000
5000
00:43
but would miss the point.
5
25000
2000
00:45
(Laughter)
6
27000
3000
00:48
He would have a great time.
7
30000
3000
00:51
And of course, if you're a speaker here, like Hans Rosling,
8
33000
5000
00:56
a speaker finds this complex, tricky. But in Hans Rosling's case,
9
38000
5000
01:01
he had a secret weapon yesterday,
10
43000
2000
01:03
literally, in his sword swallowing act.
11
45000
4000
01:07
And I must say, I thought of quite a few objects
12
49000
2000
01:09
that I might try to swallow today and finally gave up on,
13
51000
5000
01:14
but he just did it and that was a wonderful thing.
14
56000
4000
01:18
So Puck meant not only are we fools in the pejorative sense,
15
60000
5000
01:23
but that we're easily fooled. In fact, what Shakespeare
16
65000
4000
01:27
was pointing out is we go to the theater in order to be fooled,
17
69000
3000
01:30
so we're actually looking forward to it.
18
72000
4000
01:34
We go to magic shows in order to be fooled.
19
76000
2000
01:36
And this makes many things fun, but it makes it difficult to actually
20
78000
8000
01:44
get any kind of picture on the world we live in or on ourselves.
21
86000
4000
01:48
And our friend, Betty Edwards,
22
90000
2000
01:50
the "Drawing on the Right Side of the Brain" lady, shows these two tables
23
92000
3000
01:53
to her drawing class and says,
24
95000
5000
01:58
"The problem you have with learning to draw
25
100000
4000
02:02
is not that you can't move your hand,
26
104000
2000
02:04
but that the way your brain perceives images is faulty.
27
106000
6000
02:10
It's trying to perceive images into objects
28
112000
2000
02:12
rather than seeing what's there."
29
114000
2000
02:14
And to prove it, she says, "The exact size and shape of these tabletops
30
116000
5000
02:19
is the same, and I'm going to prove it to you."
31
121000
3000
02:22
She does this with cardboard, but since I have
32
124000
3000
02:25
an expensive computer here
33
127000
3000
02:28
I'll just rotate this little guy around and ...
34
130000
3000
02:34
Now having seen that -- and I've seen it hundreds of times,
35
136000
3000
02:37
because I use this in every talk I give -- I still can't see
36
139000
4000
02:41
that they're the same size and shape, and I doubt that you can either.
37
143000
5000
02:46
So what do artists do? Well, what artists do is to measure.
38
148000
5000
02:51
They measure very, very carefully.
39
153000
2000
02:53
And if you measure very, very carefully with a stiff arm and a straight edge,
40
155000
4000
02:57
you'll see that those two shapes are
41
159000
2000
02:59
exactly the same size.
42
161000
3000
03:02
And the Talmud saw this a long time ago, saying,
43
164000
5000
03:07
"We see things not as they are, but as we are."
44
169000
3000
03:10
I certainly would like to know what happened to the person
45
172000
2000
03:12
who had that insight back then,
46
174000
3000
03:15
if they actually followed it to its ultimate conclusion.
47
177000
4000
03:21
So if the world is not as it seems and we see things as we are,
48
183000
2000
03:23
then what we call reality is a kind of hallucination
49
185000
6000
03:29
happening inside here. It's a waking dream,
50
191000
3000
03:32
and understanding that that is what we actually exist in
51
194000
5000
03:37
is one of the biggest epistemological barriers in human history.
52
199000
5000
03:42
And what that means: "simple and understandable"
53
204000
2000
03:44
might not be actually simple or understandable,
54
206000
3000
03:47
and things we think are "complex" might be made simple and understandable.
55
209000
6000
03:53
Somehow we have to understand ourselves to get around our flaws.
56
215000
4000
03:57
We can think of ourselves as kind of a noisy channel.
57
219000
2000
03:59
The way I think of it is, we can't learn to see
58
221000
5000
04:04
until we admit we're blind.
59
226000
2000
04:06
Once you start down at this very humble level,
60
228000
4000
04:10
then you can start finding ways to see things.
61
232000
3000
04:13
And what's happened, over the last 400 years in particular,
62
235000
5000
04:18
is that human beings have invented "brainlets" --
63
240000
3000
04:21
little additional parts for our brain --
64
243000
4000
04:25
made out of powerful ideas that help us
65
247000
2000
04:27
see the world in different ways.
66
249000
2000
04:29
And these are in the form of sensory apparatus --
67
251000
3000
04:32
telescopes, microscopes -- reasoning apparatus --
68
254000
5000
04:37
various ways of thinking -- and, most importantly,
69
259000
4000
04:41
in the ability to change perspective on things.
70
263000
4000
04:45
I'll talk about that a little bit.
71
267000
1000
04:46
It's this change in perspective
72
268000
2000
04:48
on what it is we think we're perceiving
73
270000
3000
04:51
that has helped us make more progress in the last 400 years
74
273000
5000
04:56
than we have in the rest of human history.
75
278000
2000
04:58
And yet, it is not taught in any K through 12 curriculum in America that I'm aware of.
76
280000
8000
05:11
So one of the things that goes from simple to complex
77
293000
2000
05:13
is when we do more. We like more.
78
295000
3000
05:16
If we do more in a kind of a stupid way,
79
298000
3000
05:19
the simplicity gets complex
80
301000
3000
05:22
and, in fact, we can keep on doing it for a very long time.
81
304000
5000
05:27
But Murray Gell-Mann yesterday talked about emergent properties;
82
309000
3000
05:30
another name for them could be "architecture"
83
312000
4000
05:34
as a metaphor for taking the same old material
84
316000
4000
05:38
and thinking about non-obvious, non-simple ways of combining it.
85
320000
7000
05:45
And in fact, what Murray was talking about yesterday in the fractal beauty of nature --
86
327000
8000
05:53
of having the descriptions
87
335000
2000
05:55
at various levels be rather similar --
88
337000
4000
05:59
all goes down to the idea that the elementary particles
89
341000
5000
06:04
are both sticky and standoffish,
90
346000
3000
06:07
and they're in violent motion.
91
349000
4000
06:11
Those three things give rise to all the different levels
92
353000
3000
06:14
of what seem to be complexity in our world.
93
356000
4000
06:20
But how simple?
94
362000
2000
06:22
So, when I saw Roslings' Gapminder stuff a few years ago,
95
364000
5000
06:27
I just thought it was the greatest thing I'd seen
96
369000
2000
06:29
in conveying complex ideas simply.
97
371000
5000
06:34
But then I had a thought of, "Boy, maybe it's too simple."
98
376000
3000
06:37
And I put some effort in to try and check
99
379000
5000
06:42
to see how well these simple portrayals of trends over time
100
384000
4000
06:46
actually matched up with some ideas and investigations from the side,
101
388000
5000
06:51
and I found that they matched up very well.
102
393000
2000
06:53
So the Roslings have been able to do simplicity
103
395000
5000
06:58
without removing what's important about the data.
104
400000
4000
07:02
Whereas the film yesterday that we saw
105
404000
4000
07:06
of the simulation of the inside of a cell,
106
408000
2000
07:08
as a former molecular biologist, I didn't like that at all.
107
410000
6000
07:14
Not because it wasn't beautiful or anything,
108
416000
2000
07:16
but because it misses the thing that most students fail to understand
109
418000
5000
07:21
about molecular biology, and that is:
110
423000
3000
07:24
why is there any probability at all of two complex shapes
111
426000
5000
07:29
finding each other just the right way
112
431000
2000
07:31
so they combine together and be catalyzed?
113
433000
3000
07:34
And what we saw yesterday was
114
436000
2000
07:36
every reaction was fortuitous;
115
438000
3000
07:39
they just swooped in the air and bound, and something happened.
116
441000
4000
07:43
But in fact, those molecules are spinning at the rate of
117
445000
4000
07:47
about a million revolutions per second;
118
449000
3000
07:50
they're agitating back and forth their size every two nanoseconds;
119
452000
6000
07:56
they're completely crowded together, they're jammed,
120
458000
3000
07:59
they're bashing up against each other.
121
461000
3000
08:02
And if you don't understand that in your mental model of this stuff,
122
464000
3000
08:05
what happens inside of a cell seems completely mysterious and fortuitous,
123
467000
5000
08:10
and I think that's exactly the wrong image
124
472000
2000
08:12
for when you're trying to teach science.
125
474000
3000
08:18
So, another thing that we do is to confuse adult sophistication
126
480000
5000
08:23
with the actual understanding of some principle.
127
485000
5000
08:28
So a kid who's 14 in high school
128
490000
2000
08:30
gets this version of the Pythagorean theorem,
129
492000
6000
08:36
which is a truly subtle and interesting proof,
130
498000
3000
08:39
but in fact it's not a good way to start learning about mathematics.
131
501000
7000
08:46
So a more direct one, one that gives you more of the feeling of math,
132
508000
5000
08:51
is something closer to Pythagoras' own proof, which goes like this:
133
513000
4000
08:55
so here we have this triangle, and if we surround that C square with
134
517000
6000
09:01
three more triangles and we copy that,
135
523000
3000
09:04
notice that we can move those triangles down like this.
136
526000
5000
09:09
And that leaves two open areas that are kind of suspicious ...
137
531000
3000
09:12
and bingo. That is all you have to do.
138
534000
7000
09:19
And this kind of proof is the kind of proof
139
541000
2000
09:21
that you need to learn when you're learning mathematics
140
543000
3000
09:24
in order to get an idea of what it means
141
546000
3000
09:27
before you look into the, literally, 1,200 or 1,500 proofs
142
549000
4000
09:31
of Pythagoras' theorem that have been discovered.
143
553000
3000
09:37
Now let's go to young children.
144
559000
3000
09:40
This is a very unusual teacher
145
562000
2000
09:42
who was a kindergarten and first-grade teacher,
146
564000
4000
09:46
but was a natural mathematician.
147
568000
2000
09:48
So she was like that jazz musician friend you have who never studied music
148
570000
5000
09:53
but is a terrific musician;
149
575000
2000
09:55
she just had a feeling for math.
150
577000
2000
09:57
And here are her six-year-olds,
151
579000
3000
10:00
and she's got them making shapes out of a shape.
152
582000
5000
10:05
So they pick a shape they like -- like a diamond, or a square,
153
587000
2000
10:07
or a triangle, or a trapezoid -- and then they try and make
154
589000
3000
10:10
the next larger shape of that same shape, and the next larger shape.
155
592000
4000
10:14
You can see the trapezoids are a little challenging there.
156
596000
4000
10:18
And what this teacher did on every project
157
600000
3000
10:21
was to have the children act like first it was a creative arts project,
158
603000
5000
10:26
and then something like science.
159
608000
2000
10:28
So they had created these artifacts.
160
610000
2000
10:30
Now she had them look at them and do this ... laborious,
161
612000
4000
10:34
which I thought for a long time, until she explained to me was
162
616000
4000
10:38
to slow them down so they'll think.
163
620000
3000
10:41
So they're cutting out the little pieces of cardboard here
164
623000
3000
10:44
and pasting them up.
165
626000
2000
10:46
But the whole point of this thing is
166
628000
4000
10:50
for them to look at this chart and fill it out.
167
632000
3000
10:53
"What have you noticed about what you did?"
168
635000
4000
10:57
And so six-year-old Lauren there noticed that the first one took one,
169
639000
4000
11:01
and the second one took three more
170
643000
5000
11:06
and the total was four on that one,
171
648000
2000
11:08
the third one took five more and the total was nine on that one,
172
650000
4000
11:12
and then the next one.
173
654000
1000
11:13
She saw right away that the additional tiles that you had to add
174
655000
5000
11:18
around the edges was always going to grow by two,
175
660000
4000
11:22
so she was very confident about how she made those numbers there.
176
664000
3000
11:25
And she could see that these were the square numbers up until about six,
177
667000
5000
11:30
where she wasn't sure what six times six was
178
672000
3000
11:33
and what seven times seven was,
179
675000
2000
11:35
but then she was confident again.
180
677000
3000
11:38
So that's what Lauren did.
181
680000
2000
11:40
And then the teacher, Gillian Ishijima, had the kids
182
682000
4000
11:44
bring all of their projects up to the front of the room and put them on the floor,
183
686000
3000
11:47
and everybody went batshit: "Holy shit! They're the same!"
184
689000
8000
11:55
No matter what the shapes were, the growth law is the same.
185
697000
4000
11:59
And the mathematicians and scientists in the crowd
186
701000
3000
12:02
will recognize these two progressions
187
704000
2000
12:04
as a first-order discrete differential equation
188
706000
3000
12:07
and a second-order discrete differential equation,
189
709000
5000
12:12
derived by six-year-olds.
190
714000
4000
12:16
Well, that's pretty amazing.
191
718000
1000
12:17
That isn't what we usually try to teach six-year-olds.
192
719000
3000
12:20
So, let's take a look now at how we might use the computer for some of this.
193
722000
7000
12:27
And so the first idea here is
194
729000
4000
12:31
just to show you the kind of things that children do.
195
733000
4000
12:35
I'm using the software that we're putting on the $100 laptop.
196
737000
5000
12:40
So I'd like to draw a little car here --
197
742000
6000
12:46
I'll just do this very quickly -- and put a big tire on him.
198
748000
7000
12:59
And I get a little object here and I can look inside this object,
199
761000
4000
13:03
I'll call it a car. And here's a little behavior: car forward.
200
765000
5000
13:08
Each time I click it, car turn.
201
770000
3000
13:11
If I want to make a little script to do this over and over again,
202
773000
2000
13:13
I just drag these guys out and set them going.
203
775000
5000
13:20
And I can try steering the car here by ...
204
782000
3000
13:23
See the car turn by five here?
205
785000
2000
13:25
So what if I click this down to zero?
206
787000
3000
13:28
It goes straight. That's a big revelation for nine-year-olds.
207
790000
5000
13:33
Make it go in the other direction.
208
795000
2000
13:35
But of course, that's a little bit like kissing your sister
209
797000
2000
13:37
as far as driving a car,
210
799000
3000
13:40
so the kids want to do a steering wheel;
211
802000
3000
13:43
so they draw a steering wheel.
212
805000
3000
13:46
And we'll call this a wheel.
213
808000
5000
13:51
See this wheel's heading here?
214
813000
4000
13:55
If I turn this wheel, you can see that number over there going minus and positive.
215
817000
5000
14:00
That's kind of an invitation to pick up this name of
216
822000
2000
14:02
those numbers coming out there
217
824000
3000
14:05
and to just drop it into the script here,
218
827000
2000
14:07
and now I can steer the car with the steering wheel.
219
829000
5000
14:12
And it's interesting.
220
834000
2000
14:14
You know how much trouble the children have with variables,
221
836000
3000
14:17
but by learning it this way, in a situated fashion,
222
839000
2000
14:19
they never forget from this single trial
223
841000
3000
14:22
what a variable is and how to use it.
224
844000
3000
14:25
And we can reflect here the way Gillian Ishijima did.
225
847000
2000
14:27
So if you look at the little script here,
226
849000
2000
14:29
the speed is always going to be 30.
227
851000
2000
14:31
We're going to move the car according to that over and over again.
228
853000
5000
14:36
And I'm dropping a little dot for each one of these things;
229
858000
4000
14:40
they're evenly spaced because they're 30 apart.
230
862000
3000
14:43
And what if I do this progression that the six-year-olds did
231
865000
3000
14:46
of saying, "OK, I'm going to increase the speed by two each time,
232
868000
5000
14:51
and then I'm going to increase the distance by the speed each time?
233
873000
3000
14:54
What do I get there?"
234
876000
4000
14:58
We get a visual pattern of what these nine-year-olds called acceleration.
235
880000
7000
15:05
So how do the children do science?
236
887000
2000
15:08
(Video) Teacher: [Choose] objects that you think will fall to the Earth at the same time.
237
890000
3000
15:11
Student 1: Ooh, this is nice.
238
893000
3000
15:18
Teacher: Do not pay any attention
239
900000
2000
15:20
to what anybody else is doing.
240
902000
3000
15:35
Who's got the apple?
241
917000
2000
15:37
Alan Kay: They've got little stopwatches.
242
919000
2000
15:44
Student 2: What did you get? What did you get?
243
926000
2000
15:46
AK: Stopwatches aren't accurate enough.
244
928000
3000
15:49
Student 3: 0.99 seconds.
245
931000
2000
15:52
Teacher: So put "sponge ball" ...
246
934000
3000
15:56
Student 4l: [I decided to] do the shot put and the sponge ball
247
938000
3000
15:59
because they're two totally different weights,
248
941000
3000
16:02
and if you drop them at the same time,
249
944000
2000
16:04
maybe they'll drop at the same speed.
250
946000
2000
16:06
Teacher: Drop. Class: Whoa!
251
948000
2000
16:10
AK: So obviously, Aristotle never asked a child
252
952000
3000
16:13
about this particular point
253
955000
3000
16:16
because, of course, he didn't bother doing the experiment,
254
958000
2000
16:18
and neither did St. Thomas Aquinas.
255
960000
2000
16:20
And it was not until Galileo actually did it
256
962000
2000
16:22
that an adult thought like a child,
257
964000
3000
16:25
only 400 years ago.
258
967000
3000
16:28
We get one child like that about every classroom of 30 kids
259
970000
4000
16:32
who will actually cut straight to the chase.
260
974000
3000
16:35
Now, what if we want to look at this more closely?
261
977000
3000
16:38
We can take a movie of what's going on,
262
980000
3000
16:41
but even if we single stepped this movie,
263
983000
2000
16:43
it's tricky to see what's going on.
264
985000
2000
16:45
And so what we can do is we can lay out the frames side by side
265
987000
3000
16:48
or stack them up.
266
990000
2000
16:50
So when the children see this, they say, "Ah! Acceleration,"
267
992000
5000
16:55
remembering back four months when they did their cars sideways,
268
997000
3000
16:58
and they start measuring to find out what kind of acceleration it is.
269
1000000
6000
17:04
So what I'm doing is measuring from the bottom of one image
270
1006000
6000
17:10
to the bottom of the next image, about a fifth of a second later,
271
1012000
5000
17:15
like that. And they're getting faster and faster each time,
272
1017000
2000
17:17
and if I stack these guys up, then we see the differences; the increase
273
1019000
10000
17:27
in the speed is constant.
274
1029000
3000
17:30
And they say, "Oh, yeah. Constant acceleration.
275
1032000
2000
17:32
We've done that already."
276
1034000
2000
17:34
And how shall we look and verify that we actually have it?
277
1036000
8000
17:42
So you can't tell much from just making the ball drop there,
278
1044000
5000
17:47
but if we drop the ball and run the movie at the same time,
279
1049000
4000
17:53
we can see that we have come up with an accurate physical model.
280
1055000
5000
18:00
Galileo, by the way, did this very cleverly
281
1062000
4000
18:04
by running a ball backwards down the strings of his lute.
282
1066000
3000
18:07
I pulled out those apples to remind myself to tell you that
283
1069000
5000
18:12
this is actually probably a Newton and the apple type story,
284
1074000
5000
18:17
but it's a great story.
285
1079000
2000
18:19
And I thought I would do just one thing
286
1081000
2000
18:21
on the $100 laptop here just to prove that this stuff works here.
287
1083000
10000
18:31
So once you have gravity, here's this --
288
1093000
3000
18:34
increase the speed by something,
289
1096000
2000
18:36
increase the ship's speed.
290
1098000
3000
18:39
If I start the little game here that the kids have done,
291
1101000
3000
18:42
it'll crash the space ship.
292
1104000
2000
18:44
But if I oppose gravity, here we go ... Oops!
293
1106000
4000
18:48
(Laughter)
294
1110000
2000
18:50
One more.
295
1112000
1000
18:54
Yeah, there we go. Yeah, OK?
296
1116000
5000
18:59
I guess the best way to end this is with two quotes:
297
1121000
7000
19:06
Marshall McLuhan said,
298
1128000
2000
19:08
"Children are the messages that we send to the future,"
299
1130000
4000
19:12
but in fact, if you think of it,
300
1134000
2000
19:14
children are the future we send to the future.
301
1136000
2000
19:16
Forget about messages;
302
1138000
3000
19:19
children are the future,
303
1141000
3000
19:22
and children in the first and second world
304
1144000
2000
19:24
and, most especially, in the third world
305
1146000
3000
19:27
need mentors.
306
1149000
2000
19:29
And this summer, we're going to build five million of these $100 laptops,
307
1151000
5000
19:34
and maybe 50 million next year.
308
1156000
2000
19:36
But we couldn't create 1,000 new teachers this summer to save our life.
309
1158000
7000
19:43
That means that we, once again, have a thing where we can put technology out,
310
1165000
6000
19:49
but the mentoring that is required to go
311
1171000
3000
19:52
from a simple new iChat instant messaging system
312
1174000
5000
19:57
to something with depth is missing.
313
1179000
2000
19:59
I believe this has to be done with a new kind of user interface,
314
1181000
3000
20:02
and this new kind of user interface could be done
315
1184000
4000
20:06
with an expenditure of about 100 million dollars.
316
1188000
5000
20:11
It sounds like a lot, but it is literally 18 minutes of what we're spending in Iraq --
317
1193000
7000
20:18
we're spending 8 billion dollars a month; 18 minutes is 100 million dollars --
318
1200000
5000
20:23
so this is actually cheap.
319
1205000
2000
20:25
And Einstein said,
320
1207000
4000
20:29
"Things should be as simple as possible, but not simpler."
321
1211000
3000
20:32
Thank you.
322
1214000
1000

▲Back to top

ABOUT THE SPEAKER
Alan Kay - Educator and computing pioneer
One of the true luminaries of personal computing, Alan Kay conceived of laptops and graphical interfaces years before they were realized. At XeroxPARC, Apple, HP and Disney, he has developed tools for improving the mind.

Why you should listen

"The best way to predict the future is to invent it." Alan Kay not only coined this favorite tech-world adage, but has proven its truth several times. A true polymath, as well as inventor, he has combined engineering brilliance with knowledge of child development, epistemology, molecular biology and more.

In the 1960s, Kay joined the computer team at XeroxPARC, where he worked on world-changing inventions like the graphical interface, object-oriented programming, and the personal computer itself. Later, at Apple, Atari, HP, Disney, and now at his own nonprofits, he has helped refine the tools he anticipated long before they were realized.

As the industry has blossomed, however, Kay continues to grapple with the deeper purpose of computing, struggling to create the machine that won't only recapitulate patterns in the world as we know it but will teach both children and adults to think, to see what otherwise is beyond them.

More profile about the speaker
Alan Kay | Speaker | TED.com