ABOUT THE SPEAKER
Daniel Engber - Journalist
Daniel Engber explores science and culture as an award-winning journalist for Radiolab, the New York Times, Slate and Wired, among others.

Why you should listen

Daniel Engber's scientific method of distracting free-throw shooters in the NBA appeared in the New York Times Magazine's "Year in Ideas" and his viral website, Crying While Eating, earned spots on "The Tonight Show," VH1 and National Public Radio. He studied literature at Harvard College and neuroscience at the University of California, San Francisco and has won several awards for his writing, including the National Academies of Science Communication Award in 2012.

More profile about the speaker
Daniel Engber | Speaker | TED.com
Small Thing Big Idea

Daniel Engber: How the progress bar keeps you sane

Filmed:
605,473 views

The progress bar makes waiting more exciting... and mitigates our fear of death. Journalist Daniel Engber explores how it came into existence.
- Journalist
Daniel Engber explores science and culture as an award-winning journalist for Radiolab, the New York Times, Slate and Wired, among others. Full bio

Double-click the English transcript below to play the video.

00:12
How many people are bored at their desk
0
103
2374
00:14
for how many hours every day
1
2501
1518
00:16
and how many days a week
and how many weeks a year
2
4043
2350
00:18
for how many years in their life?
3
6417
1578
00:20
[Small thing. Big idea.]
4
8020
2584
00:22
[Daniel Engber on
the Progress Bar]
5
10629
2000
00:25
The progress bar is just
an indicator on a computer
6
13165
2692
00:27
that something's happening
inside the device.
7
15881
3026
00:31
The classic one that's been used
for years is a horizontal bar.
8
19579
4532
00:36
I mean, this goes back
to pre-computer versions of this
9
24135
3841
00:40
on ledgers, where people would fill in
a horizontal bar from left to right
10
28000
4314
00:44
to show how much of a task
they had completed at a factory.
11
32338
3616
00:47
This is just the same thing on a screen.
12
35978
2176
00:50
Something happened in the 70s
13
38497
1488
00:52
that is sometimes referred
to as "the software crisis,"
14
40009
2764
00:54
where suddenly, computers
were getting more complicated
15
42797
2908
00:57
more quickly than anyone
had been prepared for,
16
45729
3278
01:01
from a design perspective.
17
49031
1786
01:02
People were using percent-done
indicators in different ways.
18
50841
4309
01:07
So you might have a graphical
countdown clock,
19
55174
2645
01:09
or they would have a line of asterisks
20
57843
2795
01:12
that would fill out
from left to right on a screen.
21
60662
2395
01:15
But no one had done
a systematic survey of these things
22
63081
3593
01:18
and tried to figure out:
23
66698
1183
01:19
How do they actually affect
the user's experience
24
67905
3111
01:23
of sitting at the computer?
25
71040
1327
01:24
This graduate student named Brad Myers,
26
72391
1950
01:26
in 1985, decided he would study this.
27
74365
3234
01:29
He found that it didn't really matter
28
77623
2053
01:31
if the percent-done indicator
was giving you the accurate percent done.
29
79700
5106
01:36
What mattered was
that it was there at all.
30
84830
2730
01:39
Just seeing it there
made people feel better,
31
87584
2604
01:42
and that was the most surprising thing.
32
90212
2689
01:44
He has all these ideas
about what this thing could do.
33
92925
2919
01:47
Maybe it could make people
relax effectively.
34
95868
4086
01:51
Maybe it would allow people
to turn away from their machine
35
99978
4670
01:56
and do something else
of exactly the right duration.
36
104672
3063
01:59
They would look and say,
"Oh, the progress bar is half done.
37
107759
2881
02:02
That took five minutes.
38
110664
1151
02:03
So now I have five minutes
to send this fax,"
39
111839
3897
02:07
or whatever people were doing in 1985.
40
115760
2364
02:10
Both of those things are wrong.
41
118148
1804
02:11
Like, when you see that progress bar,
42
119976
1866
02:13
it sort of locks your attention
in a tractor beam,
43
121866
2967
02:16
and it turns the experience of waiting
44
124857
2016
02:18
into this exciting narrative
that you're seeing unfold in front of you:
45
126897
5082
02:24
that somehow, this time you've spent
waiting in frustration
46
132003
3849
02:27
for the computer to do something,
47
135876
1635
02:29
has been reconceptualized as:
48
137535
1975
02:31
"Progress! Oh! Great stuff is happening!"
49
139535
2294
02:33
[Progress...]
50
141853
2138
02:36
But once you start thinking
about the progress bar
51
144015
3215
02:39
as something that's more
about dulling the pain of waiting,
52
147254
3345
02:42
well, then you can start fiddling
around with the psychology.
53
150623
3883
02:46
So if you have a progress bar
that just moves at a constant rate --
54
154530
4048
02:50
let's say, that's really
what's happening in the computer --
55
158602
2976
02:53
that will feel to people
like it's slowing down.
56
161602
3257
02:57
We get bored.
57
165668
1286
03:00
Well, now you can start
trying to enhance it
58
168002
2107
03:02
and make it appear to move
more quickly than it really is,
59
170133
2762
03:04
make it move faster at the beginning,
like a burst of speed.
60
172919
3707
03:08
That's exciting, people feel like,
"Oh! Something's really happening!"
61
176650
3344
03:12
Then you can move back into a more
naturalistic growth of the progress bar
62
180018
4029
03:16
as you go along.
63
184071
1311
03:17
You're assuming that people are focusing
on the passage of time --
64
185406
3194
03:20
they're trying to watch grass grow,
65
188624
1703
03:22
they're trying to watch a pot of water,
waiting for it to boil,
66
190351
2980
03:25
and you're just trying
to make that less boring,
67
193355
2392
03:27
less painful and less frustrating
68
195771
2226
03:30
than it was before.
69
198021
1326
03:31
So the progress bar at least gives you
70
199371
3001
03:34
the vision of a beginning and an end,
71
202396
2461
03:36
and you're working towards a goal.
72
204881
1694
03:38
I think in some ways,
it mitigates the fear of death.
73
206599
2953
03:44
Too much?
74
212079
2111
Translated by Camille Martínez
Reviewed by Krystian Aparta

▲Back to top

ABOUT THE SPEAKER
Daniel Engber - Journalist
Daniel Engber explores science and culture as an award-winning journalist for Radiolab, the New York Times, Slate and Wired, among others.

Why you should listen

Daniel Engber's scientific method of distracting free-throw shooters in the NBA appeared in the New York Times Magazine's "Year in Ideas" and his viral website, Crying While Eating, earned spots on "The Tonight Show," VH1 and National Public Radio. He studied literature at Harvard College and neuroscience at the University of California, San Francisco and has won several awards for his writing, including the National Academies of Science Communication Award in 2012.

More profile about the speaker
Daniel Engber | Speaker | TED.com